This is a
copyright-compliant version of a paper published as Kinderman P (2009) Understanding
and Addressing Psychological and Social Problems: the Mediating Psychological
Processes Model. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, Volume 55, issue 5
(September 2009), p. 464-470. ISSN: 0020-7640 DOI: 10.1177/0020764008097757 http://isp.sagepub.com/content/55/5/464.short
Understanding and addressing
psychological and social problems: The mediating psychological processes model
Abstract
Background:
Psychological and social problems such as mental disorder, unemployment,
substance misuse and crime are personally distressing and absorb huge
proportions of Government effort. Addressing these is a multi-agency,
multidisciplinary exercise, but there is evidence of a marked policy shift
toward the provision of psychological therapies and interventions.
Aim:
To offer a distinctively psychological perspective on these key social and
mental health problems.
Method:
Scholarly review of the relevant literature.
Results:
This paper presents a coherent model – the mediating psychological processes
model – addressing the complex, interconnected, nature of these problems. The
mediating psychological processes model suggests that disruption or dysfunction
in psychological processes is a final common pathway in the development of
mental disorder and social problems. The model proposes that biological, social
and circumstantial factors lead to mental disorder, crime and other social
problems through their conjoint effects in influencing or disrupting relevant
psychological processes.
Conclusions:
The implications for policy, and implementation of policy, are discussed.
Keywords: psychological model, mental
disorder, crime, social exclusion
A framework for
understanding and addressing psychological and social problems: The mediating
psychological processes model
The
human and economic costs
About 450 million people worldwide suffer
from a mental or behavioural disorder. Depression alone ranks as the third
leading contributor to the global burden of diseases (World Health
Organisation, 2003a). In the UK, mental health problems represent approximately
10% of total healthcare costs (Department of Health, 2005) and result in
an estimated £23 billion of lost employment and productivity (Social Exclusion
Unit, 2003). Three of the 10 leading risk factors for physical disease are
psychological or behavioural (unsafe sex, tobacco use, alcohol use) and three
others are closely related to psychological issues (obesity, high blood
pressure and cholesterol) (World Health Organisation, 2003b). Drug misuse is
estimated to cost the UK up to £18 billion a year in social and economic costs
(Home Office, 2002). There were nearly 11 million crimes in England and Wales
in 2005-2006; costing an estimated £60 billion a year (Home Office, 2000).
Clearly these issues overlap, and not only
because some crimes are drug related and some offenders have mental health
problems. A fundamental theoretical, as well as practical, challenge is to
account for this interconnectedness.
A
psychological model
In 1977 George Engel published his
‘biopsychosocial model’ of mental illness. His aim was to challenge biomedical
paradigms of understanding human distress, and to offer a framework that would
allow psychological and social factors to play a more appropriate role in
understanding and caring for people in personal crisis. Engel commented that:
“the dominant model of disease today is biomedical, and it leaves no room
within its framework for the social, psychological and behavioural dimensions
of illness” (Engel, 1977; p130). In contrast, Engel’s biopsychosocial model suggests
that mental disorder and other medical conditions emerge as products of a whole
human system which has physical, biological, elements, but also psychosocial
systems including personal, dyadic, familial, community and societal elements
The biopsychosocial model was widely and
enthusiastically adopted by psychiatry (or at least English psychiatry) (Falloon
& Fadden, 1993), but opposition from some mainstream psychiatrists has led
to social and psychological factors often relegated to become mere moderators
of the direct causal role of biological processes (Guze, 1989). Similar models have
promoted sociological, ecological and psychological approaches (e.g. House,
2002), but are typically imprecise in the causal relationship between the bio-
psycho- and social elements.
The mediating psychological processes model
(Kinderman, 2005), in contrast, suggests that disruption or dysfunction in
psychological processes is a final common pathway in the development of mental
disorder. Psychological approaches have always separated events from the
interpretation of events. The mediating psychological processes model addresses
this issue by separating circumstantial factors from the psychological
processes that interpret, buffer, and control responses to those events. These
processes include, but are not limited to, cognitive processes. The model
proposes that biological and social factors, together with a person’s
individual experiences, lead to mental disorder through their conjoint effects
on those psychological processes.
In this model, physical, biological,
factors are clearly recognised as of causal importance in mental disorder – and
in other social issues – but achieve their effects through their effects on the
mediating (and not merely moderating) psychological processes. For example,
dopaminergic factors believed to be significant in schizophrenia are
acknowledged (as are the genetic aspects of these factors) but are seen to have
their effects on individuals through their impact on perceptual and cognitive
systems (Bentall, 2003). Similarly, serotonergic processes associated with
depression are seen to be associated with mental disorder because of their
effects on psychological processes associated with self-esteem, beliefs in
self-efficacy, motivation and expectations of reward.
The same principles apply to social and
circumstantial factors. Living in conditions of social deprivation and poverty
can indeed lead to problems such as depression – but through the effects on
psychological processes related to the disillusionment, hopelessness, and
learned helplessness which constitute a realization that one’s actions have no
effect or purpose (Evans, Saltzman & Cooperman, 2001). Being abused or
traumatized obviously leads to problems, but this association is, again,
mediated by the disruption or malformation of psychological processes - the
ways in which the children (and later the adults) appraise themselves, the
people in their lives, and the ways in which relationships and social
intercourse should be governed (Young, 1999).
The central claim of the mediating
psychological processes model presented here – that such processes constitute a
final common pathway for the emergence of mental disorder and related social
problems – should be seen as evolutionary, rather than revolutionary. The five
component elements; the experiences themselves – the distress (or ‘symptoms of
mental illness’), the biological, social and circumstantial factors and
psychological factors, are commonly referred to in psychological approaches (Read,
Mosher & Bentall, 2004). And it is not unique for psychoanalysts and
psychotherapists to argue that psychological processes are important (see, for
example, Allen, Fonagy & Bateman, 2008). The mediating psychological
processes model is different not because it incorporates new material, but in
the hierarchical relationship it proposes between these factors. Traditional
psychiatric or medical approaches to human behaviour and emotional distress
places weight on biological, synaptic changes or processes (see Guze, 1989).
Traditional social models of mental health and social problems certainly
acknowledge the role of social causal factors, but differ from psychological models
in the weight or role given to the personal, individual, cognitive, processes.
Even the biopsychosocial model differs from the medicating psychological
processes model in that, while the former implicitly gives equal weight to the
three elements of its name, the latter treats psychological issues differently.
The fact that those psychological processes are responsible for determining the
human response to the causal factors means that these processes are given a
more central role. By suggesting that disruption of psychological mechanisms is
a final common pathway in the development of problems, this approach has
significant, if subtle, implications for policy.
Implications
for mental health services
The clinical implications of this model (Kinderman
& Tai, 2006) are that formulations rather than diagnoses should predominate
clinical planning, that these formulations should detail the hypothesised
disruption to mediating psychological processes or mechanisms, that
psychological therapies should receive higher priority, and that medical,
social and even psychological interventions are most likely to be clinically
effective if they are designed on the basis of their likely beneficial impact
on underlying psychological mechanisms. Of course, this does not mean that only
psychologists should develop such formulations, Indeed, Kinderman and Tai
(2006) acknowledge that many mental health professions use such formulations,
and argue that this should continue. Of course, social workers have
traditionally had professional responsibility for many of the problems referred
to here. It is unlikely (and probably undesirable) that an army of applied
psychologists be recruited to take their place. But it does follow that the
formulations or care plans drawn up by social workers and other social care
providers should address the psychological processes – and not merely the
social and environmental circumstances – contributing to the identified
problems.
This model implies that mental health
services should be planned on the basis of need and functional outcome rather
than diagnostic categories. Identification of common mediating psychological
process, along with common antecedents and empirically demonstrable
interventions for those psychological mechanisms may offer a common language
for research and planning. In residential care, a concept of ‘hospital’ should
be avoided. The focus of specialist teams should be based on underlying
psychological principles. Services should fully embrace the recovery approach (Ralph
& Corrigan, 2005) and should facilitate genuine service user involvement.
Access should be improved to psychological therapies based on individual case
formulations and recovery models, and nurses, occupational therapists and
social workers should develop increasing competencies in psychosocial
interventions. Psychologists should offer consultation and clinical leadership,
while psychiatry should emphasise the application of medical expertise as it
assists a multidisciplinary team in the understanding and treatment of mental
disorder.
The mediating psychological processes model
may have implications for legal process too. In most jurisdictions, it is
important to determine whether a person was unable, at the time of an offence,
to understand what they were doing or if it was wrong. In the UK, this is
judged on the basis of the presence of a ‘disease of the mind’ (Butler
Committee, 1975). In a psychological model, the question whether the person
knows the difference between right or wrong is entirely sensible, but the
material issue is not one of a ‘disease of the mind’, but whether the person’s
ability to understand the difference between right and wrong was significantly
perturbed by the sources of influence outlined above. The law has always required
a very high threshold in this respect – people are presumed ‘to possess a
sufficient degree of reason to be responsible’ for their actions (Butler
Committee, 1975). From a psychological perspective, however, courts should
consider issues of diminished responsibility by examining the extent to which
the person’s normal psychological processes (relevant to the crime in question)
were disrupted or disturbed. Was the person, at the material time, very
significantly impaired in their ability to exercise normal, reasonable, judgement?
Social
problems
The mediating psychological processes model
of mental disorder is wholly applicable more widely to social problems such as
crime, antisocial behaviour, social exclusion, drug use and the like. A
transliteration of ‘mental disorder’ into ‘social problem’ seems perfectly
reasonable if one accepts that mental disorder should no longer be regarded as
similar to a disease process (Bentall, 2003).
Although different commentators may
disagree about how much variance in criminality or indeed other social problems
can be laid at the feet of biological or physical factors (most would suggest
much less than at the feet of social difficulties), it is entirely possible
that these factors are non-trivial. In the mediating psychological processes model,
this is addressed in the same manner as the biological contributors to mental
disorder. If such biological factors do impact on crime and other social
problems, the mediating psychological processes model suggests that they do so
because of the ways in which they disrupt relevant psychological processes.
The same general approach
applies, as it did in the case of mental disorder, to social factors and life
circumstances. There is no doubt that these two broad classes of causal agents
are implicated in the development of social problems (e.g. Canter & Alison,
2000; Cullen, 1984; Robinson, 2004). But clearly not everyone exposed to causal
factors such as social deprivation goes on to offend, misuse drugs or otherwise
experience social problems. The mediating psychological processes model
suggests that these individual differences are explained by the differential
ways in which these factors impact on the relevant mediating psychological
processes.
It is not surprising, therefore, that
issues of disillusionment, lack of personal efficacy or sense of agency and the
like are associated with social deprivation and abuse on the one hand and with
both social issues such as crime, drug misuse and antisocial behaviour as well
as depression and other mental disorders on the other. But this analysis does
rather beg the question of what psychological mechanisms are actually
associated with the kinds of social problems discussed here. This is not the
place to outline these in detail, but it is fair to say that a considerable
number of psychological issues – mainly ‘hard’ issues such as cognition,
memory, attention, concentration IQ and problem-solving capacity – have been
associated with key social challenges (McGuire, 2000). Social commentators and
criminologists discuss issues such as social alienation or anomie, failures in
parenting, difficulties in attachment and role models, discipline in the sense
of the learning of the consequences for behaviour. Others comment on the
possible problems some challenging young people appear to have in terms of
deferment of gratification, problem-solving, social cognition and
emotion-control. Above, in relation to mental health, the cognitive schemas
governing relationships and social intercourse were seen as key mediating
psychological mechanisms (Kinderman, 2005). This principle clearly extends
wider – to the network of relationships collectively referred to as social
capital (Baron, Field & Schuller, 2000). Indeed, it suggests how that
social capital is constructed psychologically, and how, and how functioning
communities may be developed.
These ideas raise interesting questions of
the nature or meaning of responsibility – even of ‘free will’. If our actions,
even criminal actions, are the consequences of the psychological processes that
are themselves affected by social adversity, to what extent can we be said to
be responsible for our actions? Current psychological science cannot claim to
have answers to all such questions, but it is worth noting two significant
points. First, psychologists have long recognised that people distribute
responsibility between salient causal influences when making causal
attributions (Hewstone,
1989). Second, some jurisdictions (in particular the Netherlands)
operate a ‘sliding scale’ of criminal responsibility, explicitly acknowledging
the varying level of external influence on such behaviours (and, in this model,
on psychological processes) (van der Leij, Jackson,
Malsch & Nijboer, 2002).
Multi-agency
solutions to multi-factorial problems
Many complex and difficult issues in mental
health and social care are increasingly being addressed using multi-agency
approaches. There are in the UK several major initiatives aimed at developing
multidisciplinary approaches to mental health care (Department of Health,
2004). The Care Programme Approach (a multidisciplinary plan for
mental health care) is paralleled by multi-agency services such as Child and
Adolescent Mental Health Services, ‘BEST’ Behaviour and Educational Support
Teams, and SureStart (a multi-agency attempt to address social exclusion and
child developmental difficulties in socially deprived areas). Multi-agency
approaches protecting the public from sexual predation and other serious crimes
through ‘Multi-Agency Public Protection Arrangements’, bringing together
health, prison, police, probation and social services personnel can also be
seen. This is the common currency of the UK Government’s Social Exclusion Task
Force, and similar approaches exist in most Western countries.
The mediating psychological processes model
is clearly consonant with this approach. It is axiomatic that a number of
distal causes (from the social, biological and experiential constituencies) acieve
their impact through their conjoint effects on a number of psychological processes.
It makes sense therefore, to regard reasonable interventions or solutions as
inviting a multi-agency response. The model does not, incidentally, imply that
social factors are unimportant. Quite the reverse. It suggests that identifying
and ameliorating social disadvantage is vital in addressing positive outcome.
It may be possible, as an expert psychological therapist, positively to
influence psychological mechanisms concerned with self-concept, interpersonal
relationships and motivation. But any such interventions are likely to be much
less effective than removing or addressing the social disadvantage at source.
Again, this is an evolutionary rather than
revolutionary perspective. This approach does not suggest that psychologists
hold the keys to the kingdom. Of course, there exist professional psychologists
– clinical psychologists, neuropsychologists, forensic, occupational,
counselling, health and educational psychologists. These professionals attempt
to develop and deliver psychological interventions addressed to the mediating
psychological processes themselves. Given the central and mediating role
offered to psychological processes in the model presented here, the role of
psychological expertise could be substantial. Psychologists should assist in
developing and ensuring the implementation of care plans that draw together
identified needs of the service user (Kinderman & Tai, 2006).
Psychologists should not be secondary to social policy, but should be imbedded
in it.
But the point of this model is that social
and health initiatives achieve their effects (mirroring the causation of
problems) through their positive impacts on the mediating psychological
processes. This can inform planning regardless of the activity of professional
psychologists. Parenting programmes may be welcome, but should be evaluated on
the basis of their impact on the psychological mechanisms theoretically
identified as legitimate targets – self-concept, attachment, an appreciation of
sanctions for unacceptable behaviour, or models of social problem-solving.
Psychosocial crime reduction programmes should be similarly targeted on the
psychological issues (for example social problem-solving, impulse-control, etc)
believed to mediate the route to criminality. If we wish to address, say,
addiction (to nicotine, alcohol or any other substance), the model presented
here would suggest that it is essential – whatever the mode of intervention –
to ensure that any interventions target effectively those psychological
mechanisms that maintain addictive behaviour. This clearly does not necessitate
psychological therapy. Rather it means that social policies, health education
campaigns and Government action such as taxation or legislation should be
planned on the basis of their positive effects on psychological processes. A
multiagency response is a logical extension of the mediating psychological
processes model.
Conclusions
The mediating psychological processes model
presented here offers a coherent conceptualization of the role of psychological
mechanisms in the origin of both mental disorders and a range of social
problems. The suggestion that disruption or dysfunction in psychological
processes is a final common pathway in the development of such social problems
can help to understand the causal roles played by biological, social and
circumstantial factors, in that these elements lead to problems through their
conjoint effects on mediating psychological processes or mechanisms. The
implications for research, interventions, and policy could be considerable. The
mediating psychological processes model of the development and maintenance of
personal and social problems has the potential to facilitate the development of
public policy on a wide range of issues.
References
Allen, J.G.,
Fonagy, P. & Bateman, A. (2008) Mentalizing
in Clinical Practice. Arlington, VA, American Psychiatric Publishing.
Baron, S., Field, J.
& Schuller T. (eds). (2000) Social Capital – Critical
Perspectives.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Bentall, R.P.
(2003) Madness
explained: Psychosis and human nature. London: Allen Lane.
Butler Committee.
(1975) The
Butler Committee on Mentally Abnormal Offenders. London: HMSO.
Canter, D. &
Alison, L.J. (2000) The social psychology
of crime: Groups, teams and networks. Aldershot: Dartmouth Publishing
Company/Ashgate Publishing Limited.
Cullen, F.T. (1984)
Rethinking Crime and Deviance Theory: The
emergence of a structuring tradition. Totowa, NJ: Rowman & Allanheld
Publishers.
Department of
Health. (2004) Guidance on New Ways of Working for Psychiatrists in a
Multidisciplinary and Multi-agency Context. London: Department of Health.
Department
of Health. (2005) The 2004/05 national survey of investment in mental
health services. London: Department of Health.
Engel, G.L. (1977) The
need for a new medical model: a challenge for biomedicine. Science, 196,
129-136.
Evans, G.W.,
Saltzman, H. & Cooperman, J.L. (2001) Housing quality and children's
socioemotional health. Environment and Behaviour, 33, 389-399.
Falloon, I. &
Fadden, G. (1993) Integrated mental health care. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Guze, S. (1989) Biological
psychiatry: Is there any other kind? Psychol. Med, 19, 315-323.
Hewstone, M. (1989)
Causal
attributions: From cognitive processes to collective beliefs. Oxford:
Blackwell.
Home Office. (2002)
Updated
Drug Strategy. London: Home Office.
Home Office. (Brand
S, Price R editors) (2000) Home Office Research Study 217: The economic and
social costs of crime. London: Home Office.
House, J. (2002). Understanding
Social Factors and Inequalities in Health: 20th Century Progress and 21st
Century Prospects. Journal of Health and
Social Behavior, 43;: 125-142.
Kinderman, P. &
Tai, S. (2006) Clinical implications of a psychological model of mental
disorder. Behavioural
and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 35, 1-14.
Kinderman, P.
(2005) A psychological model of mental disorder. Harv.
Rev. Psychiatry, 13, 206-217.
McGuire, J. (2000) Cognitive-Behavioural
Approaches: An Introduction to Theory and Research. London: Home
Office, London.
Ralph, R.O. &
Corrigan, P.W. (editors). (2005) Recovery in mental illness:
Broadening our understanding of wellness. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Read, J., Mosher, L.R.
& Bentall, R.P. (editors) (2004) Models of madness: psychological,
social and biological approaches to schizophrenia. New York:
Brunner-Routledge.
Robinson, M.B.
(2004) Why crime?: An integrated systems
theory of antisocial behavior. Upper Saddle River, N.J.: Pearson/Prentice
Hall.
Social Exclusion
Unit. (2003) Mental
health and social exclusion, London: Office of the Deputy Prime Minister.
van der Leij, J.B.J, Jackson, J.L., Malsch, M. & Nijboer, J.F. (2002) Residential mental
health assessment within Dutch criminal cases: a discussion. Behav.
Sci. Law, 19: 691-702.
World
Health Organization. (2003a) Investing in Mental Health. Geneva: World
Health Organization,
World
Health Organization (Wilkinson R, Marmot M editors) (2003b) Social
determinants of health: the solid facts. 2nd edition. Geneva: World Health
Organization.
Young, J.E. (1999) Cognitive
therapy for personality disorders: A schemafocused approach. Sarasota, FL:
Professional Resources.
This comment has been removed by the author.
ReplyDeleteI am truly inspired from your write-up and sharing this too with my friends and colleagues. Robi Ludwig
ReplyDeleteNice.
ReplyDelete